About | HeinOnline Law Journal Library | HeinOnline Law Journal Library | HeinOnline

49 Ct. Rev. 64 (2013)
Addressing Implicit Bias in the Courts

handle is hein.journals/ctrev49 and id is 64 raw text is: Addressing Implicit Bias
in the Courts

Fairness is a fundamental tenet of American courts. Yet,
despite substantial work by state courts to address issues of
racial and ethnic fairness,2 public skepticism that racial and
ethnic minorities receive consistently fair and equal treatment
in American courts remains widespread.3 Why?
THE INFLUENCE OF IMPLICIT ASSOCIATIONS
Perhaps one explanation may be found in an emerging body
of research on implicit cognition. During the last two decades,
new assessment methods and technologies in the fields of
social science and neuroscience have advanced research on
brain functions, providing a glimpse into what National Public
Radio science correspondent Shankar Vedantam refers to as
the hidden brain.4 Although in its early stages, this research
is helping scientists understand how the brain takes in, sorts,
synthesizes, and responds to the enormous amount of infor-
mation an individual faces on a daily basis.5 It also is provid-
ing intriguing insights into how and why individuals develop
stereotypes and biases, often without even knowing they exist.
The research indicates that an individual's brain learns over
time how to distinguish different objects (e.g., a chair or desk)
based on features of the objects that coalesce into patterns.

These patterns or schemas help the brain efficiently recognize
objects encountered in the environment. What is interesting is
that these patterns also operate at the social level. Over time,
the brain learns to sort people into certain groups (e.g., male or
female, young or old) based on combinations of characteristics
as well. The problem is when the brain automatically associ-
ates certain characteristics with specific groups that are not
accurate for all the individuals in the group (e.g., elderly indi-
viduals are frail). Scientists refer to these automatic associa-
tions as implicit-they operate behind-the-scenes without the
individual's awareness.
Scientists have developed a variety of methods to measure
these implicit attitudes about different groups, but the most
common measure used is reaction time (e.g., the Implicit
Association Test, or IAT).6 The idea behind these types of mea-
sures is that individuals will react faster to two stimuli that are
strongly associated (e.g., elderly and frail) than to two stimuli
that are less strongly associated (e.g., elderly and robust). In
the case of race, scientists have found that most European
Americans who have taken the test are faster at pairing a white
face with a good word (e.g., honest) and a black face with a bad
word (e.g., violent) than the other way around. For African-

Footnotes
1. The Open Society Institute, the State Justice Institute, and the
National Center for State Courts funded the preparation of this
article. The views expressed are those of the authors and do not
necessarily reflect the views of the funding organizations. This
article summarizes the National Center for State Courts' project
on implicit bias and judicial education. For the full report of the
project, see PAMELA M. CASEY, ROGER K. WARREN, FRED L.
CHEESMAN 11 & JENNIFER K. ELEK, HELPING COURTS ADDRESS IMPLICIT
BIAS: RESOURCES FOR EDUCATION (2012) (hereinafter HELPING
COURTS), available at http://www.ncsc.org/-/media/Files/PDF/
Topics/Gender%20and%2ORacial%2oFairness/IB report_033012.
ashx.
2. Various state-court reports of racial fairness task forces and com-
missions can be found through the National Center for State
Courts' website at http://www.ncsconline.org/Projects Initiatives/
REFI/SearchState.asp. To access the National Center for State
Courts' Interactive Database of State Programs to address race and
ethnic fairness in the courts, go to http://www.ncsconline.org
/DResearch/ref/programs.asp.
3. For example, a 1999 a national survey of public attitudes about
state courts that found 47% of Americans surveyed did not believe
that African-Americans and Latinos receive equal treatment in
America's state courts, 55% did not believe that non-English-
speaking persons receive equal treatment, and more than two-
thirds of African-Americans thought that African-Americans
received worse treatment than others in court. See NATIONAL
CENTER FOR STATE COURTS, HOW THE PUBLIC VIEWS THE STATE
COURTS: A 1999 NATIONAL SURVEY (1999), available at
http://www.ncsconline.org/WC/Publications/ResAmtPTCPublic

ViewCrtsPub.pdf. State surveys, such as the public-opinion sur-
vey commissioned by the California Administrative Office of the
Courts, report similar findings: A majority of all California
respondents stated that African-Americans and Latinos usually
receive less favorable results in court than others, approximately
two-thirds believed that non-English speakers receive less favor-
able results, and, a much higher proportion of African-Americans,
87%, thought that African-Americans receive unequal treatment.
See David B. Rottman, Trust and Confidence in the California
Courts: A Survey of the Public and Attorneys (2005), available at
http://contentdm.ncsconline.org/cgi-in/showfile.exe?CISO-
ROOT=/ ctcomm&CISOPTR=25.
4. See SHANKAR VEDANTAM, THE        HIDDEN   BRAIN: How     OUR
UNCONSCIOUS MINDS ELECT PRESIDENTS, CONTROL MARKETS, WAGE
WARS, AND SAVE OUR LIVES (2010).
5. Social-science research on implicit stereotypes, attitudes, and bias
has accumulated across several decades into a compelling body of
knowledge and continues to be a robust area of inquiry, but the
research is not without its critics. See HELPING COURTS, supra note
1, Appendix B (What Are the Key Criticisms of Implicit Bias
Research?). There is much that scientists do not yet know. This
article and the full report on which it is based are offered as a start-
ing point for courts interested in exploring implicit bias and
potential remedies, with the understanding that advances in tech-
nology and neuroscience promise continued refinement of knowl-
edge about implicit bias and its effects on decision making and
behavior.
6. See HELPING COURTS, supra note 1, Appendix B (How Is Implicit
Bias Measured), for more information on measures of implicit
bias.

64 Court Review - Volume 49

I                Pamela M. Casey, Roger K. Warren, Fred L. Cheesman, & Jennifer K. Elek'

What Is HeinOnline?

HeinOnline is a subscription-based resource containing thousands of academic and legal journals from inception; complete coverage of government documents such as U.S. Statutes at Large, U.S. Code, Federal Register, Code of Federal Regulations, U.S. Reports, and much more. Documents are image-based, fully searchable PDFs with the authority of print combined with the accessibility of a user-friendly and powerful database. For more information, request a quote or trial for your organization below.



Short-term subscription options include 24 hours, 48 hours, or 1 week to HeinOnline.

Contact us for annual subscription options:

Already a HeinOnline Subscriber?

profiles profiles most